Chronobiol Med Search

CLOSE


Chronobiol Med > Volume 5(2); 2023 > Article
Heo, Cho, and Kang: The Difference in Spectral Power Density in Sleep Electroencephalography According to the Estimation of Total Sleep Time

Abstract

Objective

The discrepancy between self-reported subjective sleep time and objective sleep time varies from person to person, and the mechanism that can explain this discrepancy is unclear. This study aimed to investigate the difference in the spectral power of sleep electroencephalography (EEG) according to the estimation of total sleep time (TST).

Methods

Of the 4,080 participants in the Sleep Heart Health Study, 2,363 participants with complete data from polysomnography, morning sleep survey, and spectral power of sleep EEG were included in the analysis. The participants were classified as normo-estimators (estimation of TST <±60 min), under-estimators (underestimation of TST ≤60 min), or over-estimators (overestimation of TST ≥60 min). A fast Fourier transformation was used to calculate the EEG power spectrum for total sleep duration within contiguous 30-s epochs of sleep. The sleep EEG spectral power was compared among the groups after adjusting for potential confounding factors, such as age, sex, proportion of participants with insomnia, apnea-hypopnea index, and TST.

Results

Of the 2,363 participants, 1,507 (63.8%), 412 (17.4%), and 444 (18.8%) were assigned to the normo-, under-, and over-estimator groups, respectively. The power spectra during total sleep differed significantly among the groups in the delta (p=0.008) and theta bands (p=0.017) after controlling for potential confounders.

Conclusion

Higher delta and theta powers were found in the under-estimators than in the over-estimators. This study suggests that differences exist in the microstructures of polysomnography-derived sleep EEG between these two groups. This study suggests that differences exist in the microstructures of polysomnography-derived sleep EEG based on differences in sleep time estimation.

INTRODUCTION

The estimation of quantity and quality of sleep is subjective. Therefore, subjective sleep duration may differ from objective measurements obtained using polysomnography (PSG) [1]. Some individuals who sleep for >6 h may report not sleeping at all [2], whereas patients with insomnia often underestimate their total sleep time (TST) [3-6]. People with sleep state misperception, also known as paradoxical insomnia, complain of insomnia without an evidence of a short sleep duration [7-10]. Underestimation of sleep is also common in the general population, especially in middle-aged adults [8,11]. Previous researchers have found that individuals with psychiatric disorders including depression and diverse medical conditions such as rheumatoid arthritis, frequently underestimate their sleep [12-17].
Conversely, others might report that they slept far more than their objective sleep time [11,18,19]. Such sleep overestimation seems to be prevalent; however, this has been relatively less reported. Previous demographic studies have consistently shown that people in the general population, especially those with short sleep durations [20], tend to overestimate their sleep [11,21,22].
The etiology of differences in sleep estimations is unclear; however, previous studies have suggested abnormalities in sleep electroencephalography (EEG), neuroendocrine function, metabolism, and activity in the important sleep-wake regulating brain regions such as the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus and limbic system [23-26]. Studies of sleep EEG have shown that differences or alterations in EEG during sleep and the microstructure of sleep can influence sleep estimation [27-29]. However, few have addressed the difference in sleep EEG according to the estimation of sleep time. Previously, we studied the relationship between subjective-objective discrepancy (SOD) and quality of life in a large data set, the Sleep Heart Health Study (SHHS) [30]. The SHHS is a qualified large cohort study equipped with data on clinical characteristics, subjective sleep time, PSG (objective sleep data), and spectral power of sleep EEG. Therefore, this data is suitable for studying the differences in spectral power of sleep EEG according to differences in sleep estimation.
This study aimed to find the difference in spectral power of sleep EEG according to the estimation of TST in a communitydwelling population.

METHODS

Study cohort and characteristics of the data

The SHHS is an open, prospective cohort study with a largescale, multi-center, and community-based dataset. It intended to study cardiovascular diseases and other conditions, such as hypertension, cerebrovascular disease, coronary heart disease, and mortality of sleep apnea among the population in the United States [31,32]. After SHHS-1, a second study that included PSG data from 4,586 SHHS participants (SHHS-2) was conducted. All participants provided written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of each participating institution (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT00005275).
We used a subset of the SHHS 2 database obtained from the National Sleep Research Resource (https://sleepdata.org/datasets/shhs). We accessed the SHHS database after acquiring a signed agreement with Brigham and Women’s Hospital. Our project was exempted from review by the IRB of Gil Medical Center (IRB No. GDIRB2018-005). This data included the clinical characteristics of the participants and sleep habit information, including frequency of insomnia, PSG measurements, and subjective sleep reports, based on a morning survey about the previous night’s sleep after PSG. The participants with insomnia symptoms (i.e., difficulty falling asleep, difficulty staying asleep, or waking up too early) occurring on ≥16 nights per month were considered as having insomnia. Participants with complete PSG data, subjective TST data measured in the morning sleep survey, and PSG with spectral power of sleep EEG were finally included in the study.

PSG

All participants underwent overnight PSG at home, as previously described, using a Compumedics P-series recording system (Compumedics, Abbotsford, Australia) [33]. The recording montage consists of a C3–A2 and C4–A1 electroencephalogram, chin electromyogram, two electrooculograms, a single-lead electrocardiogram, an airflow sensor (an oral-nasal thermistor), oxyhemoglobin saturation measurement by pulse oximetry, measurement of respiratory effort using impedance plethysmography, and data on body position. Sleep stage scoring of all nocturnal recordings was conducted by trained technicians at a centralized PSG reading center using the Rechtschaffen and Kales criteria [34]. Apnea was defined as absent or nearly absent airflow lasting for more than 10 s [35]. Hypopnea was identified as a discernible reduction lasting more than 10 s in airflow or thoracoabdominal movement (≥30% below the baseline values) [35]. As these data had various apnea–hypopnea index (AHI) based on different hypopnea criteria, we adopted the AHI scored based on the same criteria as recommended by the hypopnea rule of the 2007 version of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine manual [9].

Spectral analysis

The spectral analysis method used has been described in a previous study [36]. The C3–A2 and C4–A1 EEG recordings were sampled at 125 Hz and analyzed using the fast Fourier transform. The fast Fourier transform was conducted on a 5-s EEG segment to obtain a frequency resolution of 0.2 Hz. Each 5-s EEG segment was first windowed with a Hanning tapering window before computing the power spectra. The power content expressed as μV2 for each 30-s epoch of sleep was determined as the average power across the six 5-s segments of the sleep EEG. Power spectra were computed for each EEG frequency band: slow oscillation (0.5–1 Hz), delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), sigma (12–15 Hz), and beta (15–20 Hz). Frequencies <0.8 Hz were excluded from the analyses to control for low-frequency artifacts, such as those caused by sweating and respiration [36]. Data derived from the central EEG electrodes (i.e., [C3/A2 + C4/A1]/2) recorded over the total sleep period were used for the present analysis. For the analysis, we used absolute spectral power, which is the integral of all the power values within each frequency range. To achieve normal distributions, all absolute power data were log-transformed [37].

Group classification after the calculation of the SOD

To calculate the SOD of total sleep duration, the objective TST measured using PSG was subtracted from the subjective TST, which was recorded in the morning sleep survey after the PSG. A difference of ≥60 min in the TST was defined as marked SOD in the previous literature [7,20]. Based on this criteria, participants were classified into the following three groups: 1) the normo-estimator group (SOD, <±60 min), 2) the under-estimator group (SOD ≤-60 min), and 3) the over-estimator group (SOD ≥60 min).

Statistical analysis

Demographic, clinical, and PSG data were compared among the three groups using an analysis of variance. To compare the spectral power of sleep EEG among the groups, we employed analysis of covariance, controlling for age, sex, proportion of participants with insomnia, AHI, and TST measured using PSG, and post-hoc testing for Bonferroni-adjusted multiple comparisons. Statistical analysis was performed at a two-sided significance level of p<0.05 using SPSS for Windows (version 23, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

RESULTS

In total, 2,363 participants were included in the final study population. Table 1 shows the demographic and clinical characteristics and PSG results. The average age of the participants was 62.2 years; 1,057 (44.7%) were men. The mean subjective TST measured using the morning sleep survey was 379.1 min, and the mean SOD of TST of all participants was 3.2 min. In PSG, the mean sleep efficiency was 79.5%, and the mean AHI was 16.0 per hour.
Table 2 shows the clinical characteristics and PSG measurements of three groups according to sleep estimation and their inter-group differences. One thousand five hundred seven participants were normo-estimators (63.8%), 412 were under-estimators (17.4%), and 444 were over-estimators (18.8%). In the over-estimator group, the age and proportion of men were higher (p<0.001) than those in the other groups. In addition, the under-estimator group had the highest proportion of participants with insomnia (p<0.001).
The over-estimator group had the lowest sleep efficiency (p< 0.001), longest sleep latency (p<0.001), highest wake after sleep onset (p<0.001), and highest AHI (p<0.001) among all the groups. They also had the highest proportion of stage N1 (p<0.001) and N2 (p<0.001) and the lowest proportion of stage N3 (p<0.001) and R (p<0.001) among the groups, in terms of sleep architecture measured using PSG. In contrast, the under-estimator group had the longest TST measured by PSG (p<0.001) and longest time in bed (p<0.001) among all the groups.
The log-transformed spectral power of sleep EEG were compared among the three groups after controlling for potential confounding factors (age, sex, frequency of participants with insomnia, AHI, and TST), and this was significantly different among the groups (Table 3). The delta power of the under-estimator group was significantly higher than that of the over-estimator group (p=0.008). Additionally, the theta power of the under-estimator group was significantly higher than that of the over-estimator group (p=0.017).

DISCUSSION

This study found that the under-estimator group had higher delta and theta power (considered slow waves) than the over-estimator group did. In this study, subjective TST was significantly different among the over-estimators (447.4 min), normo-estimators (384.4 min), and under-estimators (286.0 min). In comparison, the actual TST measured by PSG was significantly different among the over-estimators (337.9 min), normo-estimators (380.7 min), and under-estimators (399.7 min), with the under-estimators having the longest sleep duration. The under-estimators had a higher sleep efficiency and a higher proportion of N3 (deep sleep) than normo-estimators and over-estimators did, not only in terms of TST but also the sleep structures measured by PSG. This seems to be consistent with the main results of this study (i.e., greater spectral power of slow-wave EEG in under-estimators).
To the best of our knowledge, only one previous study, by Lecci et al. [38], on the spectral power of sleep EEG according to the sleep estimation in the general population has been reported. They showed that the delta power was decreased and the beta power was increased in under-estimators, which is contrary to our results [38]. This inconsistent result might be due to the differences in study methods. Their study adopted the concept of Sleep Perception Index to arbitrarily categorize the top and bottom 2.5% of the participants as over-estimators and under-estimators, respectively [38]. In our data from the SHHS-2, the mean subjective TST was 379.1 min, TST measured by PSG was 375.9 min, and the mean SOD of TST was 3.2 min, indicating only a minor discrepancy. This suggests that the data in our study have a low chance of systematic error in sleep estimation.
Although our study controlled for age, sex, AHI, and TST among the three groups, other clinical characteristics between the three groups might have acted as confounding factors. The under-estimator group was younger than the over-estimator group and had fewer male participants. Deep sleep decreases as humans age; however, a previous study showed that slow-wave sleep tends to decrease more significantly with age in men than in women [39]. In addition, there was a significant difference in the proportion of participants with insomnia, AHI, and TST between the under-estimators and over-estimators. These differences in clinical characteristics between the groups might have created a bias, although these potential confounding factors were controlled for in the statistical analysis.
The findings of this study showed a greater spectral power of the slow-wave EEG band in the under-estimator group than in the over-estimator group. As our results contradict those reported in previous studies, more replication studies on this topic and establishment of data with matched clinical characteristics among various groups are needed.

NOTES

Funding Statement

The Sleep Heart Health Study (SHHS) was supported by National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute cooperative agreements U01HL53916 (University of California, Davis), U01HL53931 (New York University), U01HL53934 (University of Minnesota), U01HL53937 and U01HL64360 (Johns Hopkins University), U01HL53938 (University of Arizona), U01HL53940 (University of Washington), U01HL53941 (Boston University), and U01HL63463 (Case Western Reserve University). The National Sleep Research Resource was supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (R24 HL114473, RFP 75N92019R002). This work was also supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korean government (MSIT) (grant number: NRF-2020R1A2C1007527).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Availability of Data and Material

The datasets generated or analyzed during the current study are available in the National Sleep Research Resource (https://sleepdata.org/datasets/shhs) after acquiring a signed agreement with Brigham and Women’s Hospital.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Seung-Gul Kang. Data curation: Seung-Gul Kang, Jun-Mu Heo. Formal analysis: all authors. Methodology: Seung-Gul Kang, Seo-Eun Cho. Software: Seung-Gul Kang. Validation: Seung-Gul Kang. Writing—original draft: all authors. Writing—review & editing: Seung-Gul Kang.

Table 1.
Demographic and polysomnographic data of participants
Variable Value (n=2,363)
Demographics
Age (yr) 62.2±10.4
Sex, male 1,057 (44.7)
Body mass index (kg/m2)
Sleep habit and subjective total sleep time
Epworth Sleepiness Scale 7.5±4.2
Subjective total sleep time recorded in the morning survey after PSG (min) 379.1±86.9
PSG data
Sleep and wake time
Time in bed (min) 475.0±69.9
Total sleep time (min) 375.9±69.1
Sleep latency (min) 25.5±28.9
Sleep efficiency (%) 79.5±11.6
WASO (min) 79.1±54.6
REM sleep latency (min) 96.3±61.6
Sleep stage (%)
N1 5.6±3.9
N2 57.2±10.9
N3 16.5±10.9
R 20.9±6.4
Respiration
AHI (events per hour)* 16.0±15.7
Arousal index 18.3±10.0

Data are presented as mean±standard deviation or number (%).

* AHI: number of apneas and hypopneas with ≥3% oxygen desaturation per hour of sleep.

PSG, polysomnography; WASO, wake time after sleep onset; REM, rapid eye movement; N1, non-REM (NREM) stage 1; N2, NREM stage 2; N3, NREM stage 3; R, REM stage; AHI, apnea-hypopnea index

Table 2.
Comparison of demographic and PSG data among the three groups of participants
Variable Over-estimators (n=444) Normo-estimators (n=1,507) Under-estimators (n=412) Statistics*
Demographics
Age (yr) 66.8±9.6 61.1±10.3 61.4±10.5 F=55.66, p<0.001
Sex, male 253 (57.0) 648 (43.0) 156 (37.9) χ2=36.64, p<0.001
Body mass index (kg/m2) 27.6±4.9 28.2±4.9 28.0±5.1 F=2.26, p=0.105
Participants with insomnia 24 (5.4) 122 (8.1) 64 (15.5) χ2=30.29, p<0.001
Subjective total sleep time (min) 447.4±76.6 384.4±68.0 286.0±79.3 F=552.01, p<0.001
PSG data
Sleep and wake time
Time in bed (min) 469.0±81.8 470.7±66.3 497.5±64.2 F=26.40, p<0.001
Total sleep time (min) 337.9±76.7 380.7±64.0 399.7±61.9 F=103.20, p<0.001
Sleep latency (min) 29.1±32.9 24.4±27.6 25.6±28.6 F=3.54, p=0.029
Sleep efficiency (%) 72.6±13.6 81.2±10.7 80.6±9.6 F=105.99, p<0.001
WASO (min) 107.9±66.9 70.7±48.7 79.0±49.6 F=85.38, p<0.001
REM sleep latency (min) 99.3±68.3 93.5±56.4 103.2±71.4 F=4.38, p=0.013
Sleep stage (%)
N1 7.0±5.3 5.2±3.4 5.5±3.5 F=36.57, p<0.001
N2 59.0±12.0 56.8±10.7 57.0±10.3 F=7.08, p=0.001
N3 14.5±11.5 16.9±10.7 17.0±10.6 F=8.71, p<0.001
R 19.6±7.1 21.3±6.2 20.6±6.0 F=11.84, p<0.001
Respiration
AHI (event per hour) 18.6±17.8 15.6±15.4 14.6±14.0 F=8.45, p<0.001
Arousal index 20.6±12.3 17.7±9.6 18.0±8.0 F=14.3, p<0.001

Data are mean±standard deviation (SD) or number (%).

* analysis of variance or χ2 test.

PSG, polysomnography; WASO, wake time after sleep onset; REM, rapid eye movement; N1, non-REM (NREM) stage 1; N2, NREM stage 2; N3, NREM stage 3; R, REM stage; AHI, apnea-hypopnea index

Table 3.
Comparison of the absolute spectral power density* among the three groups of participants
Spectral bands Over-estimators Normo-estimators Under-estimators Statistics (ANCOVA)
Comparisons with significant differences in post-hoc analysis
F p
Slow oscillation (0.5–1 Hz) 1.92±0.24 1.94±0.22 1.96±0.23 1.32 0.266
Delta (1–4 Hz) 1.38±0.19 1.43±0.19 1.46±0.19 4.82 0.008 Over-estimators vs. under-estimators
Theta (4–8 Hz) 0.81±0.21 0.84±0.21 0.87±0.21 4.08 0.017 Over-estimators vs. under-estimators
Alpha (8–12 Hz) 0.54±0.24 0.57±0.23 0.59±0.22 0.87 0.418
Sigma (12–15 Hz) 0.22±0.22 0.26±0.21 0.27±0.21 0.62 0.548
Beta (15–20 Hz) -0.15±0.21 -0.15±0.19 -0.14±0.20 0.92 0.397
Activation index (beta/delta) -1.54±0.21 -1.58±0.20 -1.59±0.20 2.42 0.089

Data are mean±standard deviation.

* log-transformed spectral power density (log10 μV2);

analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) controlling for age, sex, proportion of participants with insomnia, apnea-hypopnea index, and total sleep time in polysomnography;

comparisons with significant differences are shown in post-hoc tests for Bonferroni-adjusted multiple comparisons

REFERENCES

1. Jackson CL, Patel SR, Jackson WB 2nd, Lutsey PL, Redline S. Agreement between self-reported and objectively measured sleep duration among white, black, Hispanic, and Chinese adults in the United States: multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Sleep 2018;41:zsy057.
crossref pmid pmc
2. McCall WV, Edinger JD. Subjective total insomnia: an example of sleep state misperception. Sleep 1992;15:71–73.
pmid
3. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, Guilleminault C, Zarcone VP, Spiegel R. Self-reports versus sleep laboratory findings in 122 drug-free subjects with complaints of chronic insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 1976;133:1382–1388.
crossref pmid
4. Edinger JD, Krystal AD. Subtyping primary insomnia: is sleep state misperception a distinct clinical entity? Sleep Med Rev 2003;7:203–214.
crossref pmid
5. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Physiological activation in patients with sleep state misperception. Psychosom Med 1997;59:533–540.
crossref pmid
6. Edinger JD, Fins AI. The distribution and clinical significance of sleep time misperceptions among insomniacs. Sleep 1995;18:232–239.
crossref pmid
7. Bastien CH, Ceklic T, St-Hilaire P, Desmarais F, Pérusse AD, Lefrançois J, et al. Insomnia and sleep misperception. Pathol Biol (Paris) 2014;62:241–251.
crossref pmid
8. Rezaie L, Fobian AD, McCall WV, Khazaie H. Paradoxical insomnia and subjective-objective sleep discrepancy: a review. Sleep Med Rev 2018;40:196–202.
crossref pmid
9. Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson AL, Quan SF. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2007.

10. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International classification of sleep disorders: diagnostic and coding manual. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005.

11. Park S, Park K, Shim JS, Youm Y, Kim J, Lee E, et al. Psychosocial factors affecting sleep misperception in middle-aged community-dwelling adults. PLoS One 2020;15:e0241237.
crossref pmid pmc
12. Klein E, Koren D, Arnon I, Lavie P. No evidence of sleep disturbance in post-traumatic stress disorder: a polysomnographic study in injured victims of traffic accidents. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 2002;39:3–10.
pmid
13. Rotenberg VS, Indursky P, Kayumov L, Sirota P, Melamed Y. The relationship between subjective sleep estimation and objective sleep variables in depressed patients. Int J Psychophysiol 2000;37:291–297.
crossref pmid
14. Neu D, Mairesse O, Hoffmann G, Dris A, Lambrecht LJ, Linkowski P, et al. Sleep quality perception in the chronic fatigue syndrome: correlations with sleep efficiency, affective symptoms and intensity of fatigue. Neuropsychobiology 2007;56:40–46.
crossref pmid pdf
15. Hirsch M, Carlander B, Vergé M, Tafti M, Anaya JM, Billiard M, et al. Objective and subjective sleep disturbances in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 1994;37:41–49.
crossref pmid
16. Elsenbruch S, Harnish MJ, Orr WC. Subjective and objective sleep quality in irritable bowel syndrome. Am J Gastroenterol 1999;94:2447–2452.
crossref pmid
17. Ng MC, Bianchi MT. Sleep misperception in persons with epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2014;36:9–11.
crossref pmid
18. Attarian HP, Duntley S, Brown KM. Reverse sleep state misperception. Sleep Med 2004;5:269–272.
crossref pmid
19. Trajanovic NN, Radivojevic V, Kaushansky Y, Shapiro CM. Positive sleep state misperception - a new concept of sleep misperception. Sleep Med 2007;8:111–118.
crossref pmid
20. Fernandez-Mendoza J, Calhoun SL, Bixler EO, Karataraki M, Liao D, VelaBueno A, et al. Sleep misperception and chronic insomnia in the general population: role of objective sleep duration and psychological profiles. Psychosom Med 2011;73:88–97.
crossref pmid
21. Silva GE, Goodwin JL, Sherrill DL, Arnold JL, Bootzin RR, Smith T, et al. Relationship between reported and measured sleep times: the Sleep Heart Health Study (SHHS). J Clin Sleep Med 2007;3:622–630.
pmid pmc
22. Van Den Berg JF, Van Rooij FJ, Vos H, Tulen JH, Hofman A, Miedema HM, et al. Disagreement between subjective and actigraphic measures of sleep duration in a population-based study of elderly persons. J Sleep Res 2008;17:295–302.
crossref pmid
23. Perlis ML, Giles DE, Mendelson WB, Bootzin RR, Wyatt JK. Psychophysiological insomnia: the behavioural model and a neurocognitive perspective. J Sleep Res 1997;6:179–188.
crossref pmid pdf
24. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Lin HM, Prolo P, Mastorakos G, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: clinical implications. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2001;86:3787–3794.
crossref pmid pdf
25. Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Germain A, Price JC, Miewald JM, Kupfer DJ. Functional neuroimaging evidence for hyperarousal in insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:2126–2128.
crossref pmid
26. Cano G, Mochizuki T, Saper CB. Neural circuitry of stress-induced insomnia in rats. J Neurosci 2008;28:10167–10184.
crossref pmid pmc
27. Perlis ML, Smith MT, Andrews PJ, Orff H, Giles DE. Beta/gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep 2001;24:110–117.
crossref pmid
28. Krystal AD, Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Marsh GR. NREM sleep EEG frequency spectral correlates of sleep complaints in primary insomnia subtypes. Sleep 2002;25:630–640.
pmid
29. Bianchi MT, Wang W, Klerman EB. Sleep misperception in healthy adults: implications for insomnia diagnosis. J Clin Sleep Med 2012;8:547–554.
crossref pmid pmc
30. Cho SE, Kang JM, Ko KP, Lim WJ, Redline S, Winkelman JW, et al. Association between subjective-objective discrepancy of sleeping time and healthrelated quality of life: a community-based polysomnographic study. Psychosom Med 2022;84:505–512.
crossref pmid pmc
31. Quan SF, Howard BV, Iber C, Kiley JP, Nieto FJ, O’Connor GT, et al. The sleep heart health study: design, rationale, and methods. Sleep 1997;20:1077–1085.
pmid
32. Zhang GQ, Cui L, Mueller R, Tao S, Kim M, Rueschman M, et al. The national sleep research resource: towards a sleep data commons. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2018;25:1351–1358.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
33. Redline S, Sanders MH, Lind BK, Quan SF, Iber C, Gottlieb DJ, et al. Methods for obtaining and analyzing unattended polysomnography data for a multicenter study. Sleep Heart Health Research Group. Sleep 1998;21:759–767.
pmid
34. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Washington, DC: Public Health Service, US Government Printing Office, 1968.

35. Berry RB, Budhiraja R, Gottlieb DJ, Gozal D, Iber C, Kapur VK, et al. Rules for scoring respiratory events in sleep: update of the 2007 AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events. J Clin Sleep Med 2012;8:597–619.
crossref pmid pmc
36. Zhang L, Samet J, Caffo B, Bankman I, Punjabi NM. Power spectral analysis of EEG activity during sleep in cigarette smokers. Chest 2008;133:427–432.
crossref pmid
37. Gasser T, Sroka L, Möcks J. The transfer of EOG activity into the EEG for eyes open and closed. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1985;61:181–193.
crossref pmid
38. Lecci S, Cataldi J, Betta M, Bernardi G, Heinzer R, Siclari F. Electroencephalographic changes associated with subjective under- and overestimation of sleep duration. Sleep 2020;43:zsaa094.
crossref pmid pdf
39. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Normal human sleep: an overview. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia: Elsevier,, 2005, p. 13–23.

TOOLS
Share :
Facebook Twitter Linked In Google+ Line it
METRICS Graph View
  • 0 Crossref
  •   Scopus 
  • 1,190 View
  • 24 Download
Related articles in Chronobiol Med


ABOUT
ARTICLE CATEGORY

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
EDITORIAL POLICIES
FOR CONTRIBUTORS
Editorial Office
RN1611, 725, Suseo-Dong, Gangnam-Gu, Seoul 06367, Republic of Korea
Tel: +82-2-445-1611    Fax: +82-2-445-1633    E-mail: editor@chronobiologyinmedicine.org                

Copyright © 2024 by Korean Academy of Sleep Medicine. All rights reserved.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next